Sensing the sky: how top-down mechanisms improve colour constancy — ASN Events

Sensing the sky: how top-down mechanisms improve colour constancy (#454)

Adrian Dyer 1 , Andrew Greentree 2 , Yu-Shan Hung 3 , Jair Garcia 1
  1. School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
  2. School of Applied Sciences, RMIT University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
  3. Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, National Vision Research Institute, Carlton, Victoria, Australia

Colour constancy, the ability to perceive colours as being equivalent independent of changes in illumination, is a property assumed essential for colour vision. Colour constancy has been evidenced in various animals including: honeybees,hawkmoths,2 goldfish,3 primates 4  and humans,5 and the potential mechanism of chromatic adaptation has been included in commonly used colour vision models 6 7 8 9 10 by applying an elegant mathematical formulation first proposed by von Kries more than a century ago.11 Indeed, chromatic adaptation shows evidence of being an important contributing mechanism of colour constancy in bees.1 12 Nevertheless, recent behavioural data from bees also suggests that these animals can directly perceive changes in the ambient illumination 13 and use such information in complex environments. 14 Furthermore, evidence of the capacity of bees to use contextual information about illumination colour to make behavioural choices 15 suggests a possibility that ‘top-down’ mechanisms may also take part in solving difficult colour discrimination tasks. We addressed this question by modelling a visual system where a top-down mechanism, expressed as a priori  information about the spectral characteristic of the ambient illumination, is used for estimating the coefficients required by the von Kries constancy model. Simulations predict that an a priori, biologically relevant guiding estimate of the ambient illumination provided to the visual system significantly improves colour constancy of flower targets when considering most conditions of daylight  (Figure). Interestingly, our model also predicts significant failures in the chromatic adaptation process at low correlated colour temperatures as those observed in bees when performing colour discrimination tasks under artificial, tungsten light. 1 12 We discuss plausible physiological mechanisms facilitating top-down processing, and how such solutions could lead to bio-inspired developments to improve colour constancy algorithms required to be implemented in digital solutions for imaging in natural conditions. 16

1758-maxell_triangles_plot_only_flowers.jpg Figure:Predicted colour shift for different flowers without (a) and assuming top-down information (b). Colour space makes no assumptions about opponent mechanisms.17

  1. Neumeyer C. 1981 Chromatic adaptation in the honeybee: Successive color contrast and color constancy. J Comp Physiol A 144(4), 543-553. (doi:10.1007/bf01326839).
  2. Balkenius A., Kelber A. 2004 Colour constancy in diurnal and nocturnal hawkmoths. J Exp Biol 207(19), 3307-3316. (doi:10.1242/jeb.01158).
  3. Dörr S., Neumeyer C. 2000 Color constancy in goldfish: The limits. J Comp Physiol A 186(9), 885-896. (doi:10.1007/s003590000141).
  4. Gegenfurtner K.R. 2003 Cortical mechanisms of colour vision. Nat Rev Neurosci 4(7), 563-572.
  5. Hurlbert A. 2007 Colour constancy. Curr Biol 17(21), R906-R907. (doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.022).
  6. Backhaus W. 1991 Color opponent coding in the visual system of the honeybee. Vision Res 31(7), 1381-1397.
  7. MacAdam D.L. 1981 Color Measurement: Theme and Variations. Berlin, Germany, Springer-Verlag.
  8. Chittka L. 1992 The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J Comp Physiol A 170(5), 533-543.
  9. Endler J.A., Mielke P.W. 2005 Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biol J Linn Soc 86(4), 405-431. (doi:doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2005.00540.x).
  10. Vorobyev M., Osorio D. 1998 Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 265, 351-358.
  11. von Kries J. 1970 Chromatic Adaptation. In Sources of Color Science (ed. MacAdam D.L.), pp. 109-119. Cambridge, MIT Press.
  12. Dyer A.G., Chittka L. 2004 Biological significance of distinguishing between similar colours in spectrally variable illumination: Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) as a case study. J Comp Physiol A 190(2), 105-114. (doi:10.1007/s00359-003-0475-2).
  13. Dyer A.G. 2006 Bumblebees directly perceive variations in the spectral quality of illumination. J Comp Physiol A 192(3), 333-338.
  14. Arnold S.E.J., Chittka L. 2012 Illumination preference, illumination constancy and colour discrimination by bumblebees in an environment with patchy light. J Exp Biol 215(13), 2173-2180.
  15. Lotto R.B., Chittka L. 2005 Seeing the light: Illumination as a contextual cue to color choice behavior in bumblebees. P Natl Acad Sci USA 102(10), 3852-3856.
  16. Garcia J.E., Greentree A.D., Shrestha M., Dorin A., Dyer A.G. 2014 Flowers through the lens: Quantitative measurement with visible and ultraviolet digital photography. PLOS ONE 9(5), e96646.
  17. Kelber A., Vorobyev M., Osorio D. 2003 Animal colour vision - behavioural test and physiological concepts. Biol Rev 78, 81-118.